Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 50
Filtrar
1.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 45(6): e26681, 2024 Apr 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38656060

RESUMEN

Olfactory perception depends not only on olfactory inputs but also on semantic context. Although multi-voxel activity patterns of the piriform cortex, a part of the primary olfactory cortex, have been shown to represent odor perception, it remains unclear whether semantic contexts modulate odor representation in this region. Here, we investigated whether multi-voxel activity patterns in the piriform cortex change when semantic context modulates odor perception and, if so, whether the modulated areas communicate with brain regions involved in semantic and memory processing beyond the piriform cortex. We also explored regional differences within the piriform cortex, which are influenced by olfactory input and semantic context. We used 2 × 2 combinations of word labels and odorants that were perceived as congruent and measured piriform activity with a 1-mm isotropic resolution using 7T MRI. We found that identical odorants labeled with different words were perceived differently. This labeling effect was observed in multi-voxel activity patterns in the piriform cortex, as the searchlight decoding analysis distinguished identical odors with different labels for half of the examined stimulus pairs. Significant functional connectivity was observed between parts of the piriform cortex that were modulated by labels and regions associated with semantic and memory processing. While the piriform multi-voxel patterns evoked by different olfactory inputs were also distinguishable, the decoding accuracy was significant for only one stimulus pair, preventing definitive conclusions regarding the locational differences between areas influenced by word labels and olfactory inputs. These results suggest that multi-voxel patterns of piriform activity can be modulated by semantic context, possibly due to communication between the piriform cortex and the semantic and memory regions.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Odorantes , Percepción Olfatoria , Corteza Piriforme , Semántica , Humanos , Masculino , Corteza Piriforme/fisiología , Corteza Piriforme/diagnóstico por imagen , Percepción Olfatoria/fisiología , Femenino , Adulto , Adulto Joven
2.
Commun Med (Lond) ; 2: 34, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35603293

RESUMEN

Background: Key to curtailing the COVID-19 pandemic are wide-scale screening strategies. An ideal screen is one that would not rely on transporting, distributing, and collecting physical specimens. Given the olfactory impairment associated with COVID-19, we developed a perceptual measure of olfaction that relies on smelling household odorants and rating them online. Methods: Each participant was instructed to select 5 household items, and rate their perceived odor pleasantness and intensity using an online visual analogue scale. We used this data to assign an olfactory perceptual fingerprint, a value that reflects the perceived difference between odorants. We tested the performance of this real-time tool in a total of 13,484 participants (462 COVID-19 positive) from 134 countries who provided 178,820 perceptual ratings of 60 different household odorants. Results: We observe that olfactory ratings are indicative of COVID-19 status in a country, significantly correlating with national infection rates over time. More importantly, we observe indicative power at the individual level (79% sensitivity and 87% specificity). Critically, this olfactory screen remains effective in participants with COVID-19 but without symptoms, and in participants with symptoms but without COVID-19. Conclusions: The current odorant-based olfactory screen adds a component to online symptom-checkers, to potentially provide an added first line of defense that can help fight disease progression at the population level. The data derived from this tool may allow better understanding of the link between COVID-19 and olfaction.

3.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 119(21): e2114966119, 2022 05 24.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35584113

RESUMEN

How the human brain translates olfactory inputs into diverse perceptions, from pleasurable floral smells to sickening smells of decay, is one of the fundamental questions in olfaction. To examine how different aspects of olfactory perception emerge in space and time in the human brain, we performed time-resolved multivariate pattern analysis of scalp-recorded electroencephalogram responses to 10 perceptually diverse odors and associated the resulting decoding accuracies with perception and source activities. Mean decoding accuracies of odors exceeded the chance level 100 ms after odor onset and reached maxima at 350 ms. The result suggests that the neural representations of individual odors were maximally separated at 350 ms. Perceptual representations emerged following the decoding peak: unipolar unpleasantness (neutral to unpleasant) from 300 ms, and pleasantness (neutral to pleasant) and perceptual quality (applicability to verbal descriptors such as "fruity" or "flowery") from 500 ms after odor onset, with all these perceptual representations reaching their maxima after 600 ms. A source estimation showed that the areas representing the odor information, estimated based on the decoding accuracies, were localized in and around the primary and secondary olfactory areas at 100 to 350 ms after odor onset. Odor representations then expanded into larger areas associated with emotional, semantic, and memory processing, with the activities of these later areas being significantly associated with perception. These results suggest that initial odor information coded in the olfactory areas (<350 ms) evolves into their perceptual realizations (300 to >600 ms) through computations in widely distributed cortical regions, with different perceptual aspects having different spatiotemporal dynamics.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Encéfalo , Odorantes , Percepción Olfatoria , Encéfalo/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Emociones , Humanos , Memoria , Olfato
4.
Chem Senses ; 2020 May 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32441744

RESUMEN

In response to the COVID-19 pandemic, countries have implemented various strategies to reduce and slow the spread of the disease in the general population. For countries that have implemented restrictions on its population in a step-wise manner, monitoring of COVID-19 prevalence is of importance to guide decision on when to impose new, or when to abolish old, restrictions. We are here determining whether measures of odor intensity in a large sample can serve as one such measure. Online measures of how intense common household odors are perceived and symptoms of COVID-19 were collected from 2440 Swedes. Average odor intensity ratings were then compared to predicted COVID-19 population prevalence over time in the Swedish population and were found to closely track each other (r=-0.83). Moreover, we found that there was a large difference in rated intensity between individuals with and without COVID-19 symptoms and number of symptoms was related to odor intensity ratings. Finally, we found that individuals progressing from reporting no symptoms to subsequently reporting COVID-19 symptoms demonstrated a large drop in olfactory performance. These data suggest that measures of odor intensity, if obtained in a large and representative sample, can be used as an indicator of COVID-19 disease in the general population. Importantly, this simple measure could easily be implemented in countries without widespread access to COVID-19 testing or implemented as a fast early response before wide-spread testing can be facilitated.

5.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 50(5): 1809-1815, 2020 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30078095

RESUMEN

The elucidation of odour awareness in children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) is important. We compared the odour awareness of young children with ASD with those of typical development (TD) children using the Children's Olfactory Behavior in Everyday Life (COBEL) questionnaire, which is a self-report measure that mainly assesses odour awareness. Forty-five young boys (aged 5-6 years), including 20 children with ASD and 25 TD children, participated in this study. The total COBEL score of the young children with ASD was lower than that of the TD children (p < 0.01). Moreover, the total COBEL score was significantly correlated with the total VABS II score (p < 0.05). Our results improve understanding of the odour awareness in children with ASD.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista/psicología , Percepción Olfatoria , Niño , Preescolar , Humanos , Masculino , Odorantes/análisis , Autoinforme , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
6.
Chem Senses ; 45(1): 37-44, 2020 01 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31711116

RESUMEN

Atypical sensory reactivities are pervasive among people with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). With respect to olfaction, most previous studies have used psychophysical or questionnaire-based methodologies; thus, the neural basis of olfactory processing in ASD remains unclear. This study aimed to determine the stages of olfactory processing that are altered in ASD. Fourteen young adults with high-functioning ASD (mean age, 21 years; 3 females) were compared with 19 age-matched typically developing (TD) controls (mean age, 21 years; 4 females). Olfactory event-related potentials (OERPs) for 2-phenylethyl alcohol-a rose-like odor-were measured with 64 scalp electrodes while participants performed a simple odor detection task. Significant group differences in OERPs were found in 3 time windows 542 ms after the stimulus onset. The cortical source activities in these time windows, estimated using standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography, were significantly higher in ASD than in TD in and around the posterior cingulate cortex, which is known to play a crucial role in modality-general cognitive processing. Supplemental Bayesian analysis provided substantial evidence for an alteration in the later stages of olfactory processing, whereas conclusive evidence was not provided for the earlier stages. These results suggest that olfactory processing in ASD is altered at least at the later, modality-general processing stage.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista/fisiopatología , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/psicología , Potenciales Evocados , Olfato/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/diagnóstico , Electrodos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Odorantes/análisis , Alcohol Feniletílico/análisis , Adulto Joven
7.
Psychoneuroendocrinology ; 106: 206-215, 2019 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31003137

RESUMEN

Unpleasant odors impair our mood and may affect physical health, even when the odorants are not toxic. A possible cause for such negative effects is stress induced by odors; however, whether the unpleasantness itself elicited stress or not has not been clear. Thus, we examined whether unpleasantness of odors induced the stress responses of emotion, the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, and the sympathetic nervous system (SNS). Six experiments were conducted, where salivary cortisol or salivary alpha amylase (sAA), markers for activities of the HPA and the SNS, respectively, were measured, along with subjective ratings of odors and emotion. First, the responses to three malodors listed in the Offensive Odor Control Law in Japan were examined. While these odors were rated as unpleasant, and exposure to them increased anxiety, no response of the HPA was observed (experiment 1, n = 69). In contrast, an increase of the SNS activity was observed after exposure to two of the three malodors, while the SNS did not respond to pleasant odors (experiments 2-4, n = 35, 34 and 30). To examine the effect of unpleasantness further, the SNS response was examined while subjective unpleasantness of odors was manipulated by adding negative verbal information (experiment 5, n = 92), or by mixing in a pleasant odor (experiment 6, n = 35). The SNS responses upon inhalation of the same odorous substances were found to be dependent on whether they were perceived as unpleasant. Finally, a correlation analysis on the pooled data from experiments 2-6 showed that the odor-elicited SNS activity and anxiety were strongly correlated with perceived unpleasantness of odors. These results suggest that subjective unpleasantness of odors per se can induce the stress response of emotion and the SNS.


Asunto(s)
Emociones/fisiología , alfa-Amilasas Salivales/metabolismo , Olfato/fisiología , Estrés Psicológico/metabolismo , Adulto , Afecto/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Hidrocortisona/análisis , Sistema Hipotálamo-Hipofisario/metabolismo , Japón , Masculino , Odorantes/análisis , Sistema Hipófiso-Suprarrenal/metabolismo , Saliva/química , Sistema Nervioso Simpático/metabolismo , Sistema Nervioso Simpático/fisiología , Adulto Joven , alfa-Amilasas/metabolismo
8.
Sci Rep ; 9(1): 4953, 2019 03 20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30894641

RESUMEN

The capacity to pay attention is important for the cognitive ability, for example, evaluating an object for its qualities. Attention can selectively prioritize the neural processes that are relevant to a given task. Neuroimaging investigations on human attention are primarily focused on vision to the exclusion of other sensory systems, particularly olfaction. Neural underpinnings of human olfactory attention are still not clearly understood. Here, we combined electroencephalographic measurements of olfactory event related potential with electrical neuroimaging to investigate how the neural responses after inhaling the same odor differ between conditions with varying levels of attention, and, in which brain areas. We examined the neural responses when participants attended to a rose-like odor of phenylethyl alcohol for evaluating its pleasantness versus its passive inhalation. Our results gathered significant evidence for attentional modulation of the olfactory neural response. The most prominent effect was found for the late positive component, P3, of olfactory event related potential within a second from the odor onset. The source reconstruction of this data revealed activations in a distributed network of brain regions predominantly in inferior frontal cortex, insula, and  inferior temporal gyrus. These results suggest that the neuronal modulations from attention to olfactory pleasantness may be subserved by this network.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Percepción Olfatoria/fisiología , Placer/fisiología , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Corteza Cerebral/diagnóstico por imagen , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Neuroimagen , Odorantes , Adulto Joven
9.
Case Rep Gastroenterol ; 12(2): 546-550, 2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30283291

RESUMEN

Lorazepam is a benzodiazepine derivative that is globally used for the therapy of anxiety and insomnia. A 51-year-old Japanese man with yellowish discoloration of the eyes and skin and pruritus was admitted due to liver dysfunction. He had taken lorazepam approximately 5 months prior to this admission. The clinical presentation and pathologic findings in the liver were consistent with drug-induced liver injury. After cessation of lorazepam, treatment with Stronger neo-minophagen C and ursodeoxycholic acid was started, and his liver injury resolved after 59 days. This case must serve as a warning to physicians to be aware of the possibility of unexpected liver injury caused by lorazepam.

10.
Methods Mol Biol ; 1820: 221-228, 2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29884948

RESUMEN

Olfactory traits in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are considered the strongest predictors of social impairment. Compared to other sensory abnormalities, olfactory abnormalities in individuals with ASD are poorly understood. In this chapter, we provide an overview of the current assessment in individuals with ASD. Several confounding factors have to be considered when conducting research on olfaction in individuals with ASD. Qualitative measures of olfaction contain only limited information about the olfactory stimuli. In addition, little systematic information is available about individual's actual uses of olfaction in daily life. Only a limited number of experimental studies have performed quantitative measurements of olfactory abnormalities in ASD. Therefore, clarifying the relationship between olfactory traits and the influence of real-life situations in a laboratory setting is very difficult. Some new methodologies for measuring olfactory traits are gradually becoming available. New methods that reveal important links between ASD and olfactory traits should be developed in the future.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista/fisiopatología , Olfato , Adolescente , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
11.
PLoS One ; 11(5): e0154392, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27138751

RESUMEN

Parental caregiving is critical for the survival of our young and continuation of our species. In humans, visual and auditory signals from offspring have been shown to be potent facilitators of parenting. However, whether odors emitted by our young also influence human parenting remains unclear. To explore this, we conducted a series of questionnaire surveys targeting parents with children under 6 years old. First, we collected episodes on experiencing odors/sniffing various parts of a child's body (n = 507). The prevalence of experiencing events described in those episodes was examined in a separate survey (n = 384). Based on those results, the Child Odor in Parenting scale (COPs) was developed, and subsequently used in the main survey (n = 888). We found COPs to have adequate content validity, concurrent validity, and reliability. Responses to the COPs demonstrated that parents, especially mothers with infants, are aware of odors from their offspring, and actively seek them in daily child-rearing. The factor structure and content of the COPs items indicated that child odors have both affective and instrumental roles. Affective experiences induce loving feeling and affectionate sniffing, while instrumental experiences pertain to specific hygienic needs. The head was the most frequent source of affective experiences, and the child's bottom of instrumental. Each was experienced by more than 90% of the mothers with a child below 1 year of age. Affective experiences significantly declined as the child grew older, possibly associated with the decline of physical proximity between parents and child. This age-related decline was not prominent for instrumental experiences, except for the bottom, which significantly declined after 3 years of age. The present findings suggest that child odors play roles in human parenting, and that their nature and significance change during the course of a child's development.


Asunto(s)
Odorantes , Responsabilidad Parental/psicología , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino , Análisis de Regresión
12.
Patient Educ Couns ; 98(5): 660-8, 2015 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25739344

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: To examine the relationship between health literacy (HL), health information access, health behavior, and health status in Japanese people. METHODS: A questionnaire survey was conducted at six healthcare facilities in Japan. Eligible respondents aged 20-64 years (n=1218) were included. Path analysis with structural equation modeling was performed to test the hypothesis model linking HL to health information access, health behavior, and health status. RESULTS: The acceptable fitting model indicated that the pathways linking HL to health status consisted of two indirect paths; one intermediated by health information access and another intermediated by health behavior. Those with higher HL as measured by the 14-item Health Literacy Scale (HLS-14) were significantly more likely to get sufficient health information from multiple sources, less likely to have risky habits of smoking, regular drinking, and lack of exercise, and in turn, more likely to report good self-rated health. CONCLUSION: HL was significantly associated with health information access and health behavior in Japanese people. HL may play a key role in health promotion, even in highly educated countries like Japan. PRACTICE IMPLICATIONS: In order to enhance the effects of health promotion interventions, health professionals should aim at raising HL levels of their target population groups.


Asunto(s)
Acceso a la Información , Pueblo Asiatico/estadística & datos numéricos , Conductas Relacionadas con la Salud , Alfabetización en Salud , Estado de Salud , Adulto , Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas , Ejercicio Físico , Femenino , Conocimientos, Actitudes y Práctica en Salud , Promoción de la Salud , Humanos , Japón , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Fumar , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
13.
Reprod Health ; 12: 10, 2015 Jan 31.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25638172

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: A recent survey of 79 countries showed that fertility knowledge was lower in Japan than in any other developed country. Given the fertility decline in Japan and the importance of fertility knowledge, we conducted an online survey to examine fertility knowledge and the related factors for effective public education. METHODS: We studied people aged 18-59 years old, n = 4,328 (the "General" group), and also people who had been trying to conceive for at least six months, 18-50 years old, n = 618 (the "Triers" group). Fertility knowledge was assessed using the Japanese version of the 13-item Cardiff Fertility Knowledge Scale (CFKS-J). All participants provided socio-demographic and fertility information. Participants also completed a 14-item health literacy scale and an 11-item health numeracy scale. We asked participants who were aware of age-related decline in fertility when and where they first acquired that knowledge. RESULTS: The average percentages of CFKS-J items answered correctly were 53.1% in the Triers group and 44.4% in the General group (p < 0.001). Multivariate linear regression models showed in the Triers group greater fertility knowledge was associated with greater health literacy and prior medical consultation regarding their fertility. In the General group greater fertility knowledge was associated with being female, younger, university educated, currently trying to conceive, non-smoking, having higher household income, having higher health literacy and having higher health numeracy. Of those who were aware of the age-related decline in fertility, around 3% first learned the fact "at school", and around 65% first learned it "through mass media" or "via the Internet". More than 30% of the respondents first learned it "less than 5 years before" the survey. CONCLUSIONS: Although fertility knowledge had improved since a previous study, possibly due to recent media coverage of age-related infertility, it was still low. Educational interventions, both in schools and in the community, may be needed to increase fertility knowledge in the general population because most people obtain fertility knowledge from mass media, which has been shown to often present distorted and inaccurate fertility information.


Asunto(s)
Fertilidad , Conocimientos, Actitudes y Práctica en Salud , Adolescente , Adulto , Estudios Transversales , Femenino , Educación en Salud , Alfabetización en Salud , Humanos , Japón , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Psicometría , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Factores Socioeconómicos , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
14.
Bioorg Med Chem Lett ; 25(5): 1086-91, 2015 Mar 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25655721

RESUMEN

We herein describe the results of further evolution of GSK-3ß inhibitors for Alzheimer's disease from our promising compounds with in vivo tau phosphorylation inhibitory activity by oral administration. Introduction of a low alkyl group instead of the phenyl group at the 3-position of the morpholine moiety aiming to improve pharmacokinetic profiles resulted in potent low molecular weight GSK-3ß inhibitors with good in vitro pharmacokinetic profiles, which also showed in vivo tau phosphorylation inhibitory activity by oral administration. Effect of the stereochemistry of the alkyl moiety is also discussed using docking models.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer/enzimología , Glucógeno Sintasa Quinasa 3/antagonistas & inhibidores , Morfolinas/química , Morfolinas/farmacología , Pirimidinas/química , Pirimidinas/farmacología , Administración Oral , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/tratamiento farmacológico , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/metabolismo , Animales , Descubrimiento de Drogas , Glucógeno Sintasa Quinasa 3/metabolismo , Glucógeno Sintasa Quinasa 3 beta , Humanos , Ratones , Simulación del Acoplamiento Molecular , Morfolinas/administración & dosificación , Morfolinas/farmacocinética , Fosforilación/efectos de los fármacos , Pirimidinas/administración & dosificación , Pirimidinas/farmacocinética , Proteínas tau/metabolismo
15.
J Huazhong Univ Sci Technolog Med Sci ; 34(5): 663-671, 2014 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25318875

RESUMEN

It remains unclear whether language tasks in one's first (L1) or second (L2) language can cause stress responses and whether frontal, autonomic and behavioral responses to stressful tasks are correlated. In this study, we studied 22 Chinese subjects whose L2 was English and measured the cerebral blood oxygenation in their frontal lobe by using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) as participants engaged in a mental arithmetic task (MAT) and verbal fluency tasks (VFTs) in L1 (Chinese) and L2 (English). To examine the activated cortical areas, we estimated the channel location based on Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) standard brain space by using a probabilistic estimation method. We evaluated heart rate (HR) changes to analyze autonomic nervous system (ANS) functioning. We found that the MAT and VFTs induced greater increases in HR than did the control (Ctrl) task. Furthermore, subjects developed greater increases in HR in the MAT and VFTL2 than they did in the VFTL1. Compared with the Ctrl task, the MAT and both VFTL1 and VFTL2 produced robust and widespread bilateral activation of the frontal cortex. Interestingly, partial correlation analysis indicated that the activity in the left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG) [Brodmann's area (BA) 47] was consistently correlated with the increases in HR across the three tasks (MAT, VFTL2, and VFTL1), after controlling for the performance data. The present results suggested that a VFT in L2 may be more stressful than in L1. The LIFG may affect the activation of the sympathetic system induced by stressful tasks, including MATs and VFTs.


Asunto(s)
Sistema Nervioso Autónomo/fisiología , Lenguaje , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional , Frecuencia Cardíaca/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Oxihemoglobinas/metabolismo , Corteza Prefrontal/metabolismo , Espectroscopía Infrarroja Corta/métodos , Estrés Psicológico
16.
Environ Health Prev Med ; 19(4): 295-306, 2014 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24810206

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: To determine the reading comprehension of health checkup reports in the context of health literacy (HL) in Japanese people. METHODS: A web-based survey was conducted among 424 Japanese adults aged 35-59 years. Participants were asked to read specifically designed health checkup reports and then answer a series of questions to examine whether they accomplished the fundamental purposes of health checkup reports (recognition of the problems, recognition of the risk of illness, recognition of the need for preventive action, and motivation for preventive action). HL was simultaneously measured using the 14-item health literacy scale (HLS-14), the 11-item Lipkus scale (Lipkus-J), and the Newest Vital Sign (NVS-J). RESULTS: About 70 % of the study subjects misread the normal/abnormal classification for at least one items. Those with lower HLS-14 scores were significantly less likely to recognize the problems, the risk of illness, and the need for preventive action for the examinee, and also less likely to express their willingness to take preventive action in compliance with the doctor's advice after having received the health checkup report. Compared with the HLS-14 scores, the Lipkus-J and NVS-J scores showed hardly any association with the reading comprehension of health checkup reports. CONCLUSION: All examinees do not always have an adequate level of HL. HL may be the major determinant of reading comprehension of health checkup reports. For more effective health checkups, health promotion service providers should become aware of the existence of examinees with inadequate HL and address the problem of misreading health checkup results.


Asunto(s)
Conocimientos, Actitudes y Práctica en Salud , Alfabetización en Salud , Adulto , Comprensión , Femenino , Humanos , Japón , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Lectura
17.
PLoS One ; 9(4): e94582, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24762459

RESUMEN

Health literacy (HL) refers to the ability to obtain, process, and understand basic health information and services, and is thus needed to make appropriate health decisions. The Newest Vital Sign (NVS) is comprised of 6 questions about an ice cream nutrition label and assesses HL numeracy skills. We developed a Japanese version of the NVS (NVS-J) and evaluated the validity and reliability of the NVS-J in patients with chronic pain. The translation of the original NVS into Japanese was achieved as per the published guidelines. An observational study was subsequently performed to evaluate the validity and reliability of the NVS-J in 43 Japanese patients suffering from chronic pain. Factor analysis with promax rotation, using the Kaiser criterion (eigenvalues ≥1.0), and a scree plot revealed that the main component of the NVS-J consists of three determinative factors, and each factor consists of two NVS-J items. The criterion-related validity of the total NVS-J score was significantly correlated with the total score of Ishikawa et al.'s self-rated HL Questionnaire, the clinical global assessment of comprehensive HL level, cognitive function, and the Brinkman index. In addition, Cronbach's coefficient for the total score of the NVS-J was adequate (alpha = 0.72). This study demonstrated that the NVS-J has good validity and reliability. Further, the NVS-J consists of three determinative factors: "basic numeracy ability," "complex numeracy ability," and "serious-minded ability." These three HL abilities comprise a 3-step hierarchical structure. Adequate HL should be promoted in chronic pain patients to enable coping, improve functioning, and increase activities of daily living (ADLs) and quality of life (QOL).


Asunto(s)
Alfabetización en Salud , Anciano , Analgésicos Opioides/uso terapéutico , Dolor Crónico/tratamiento farmacológico , Femenino , Humanos , Japón , Funciones de Verosimilitud , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Evaluación de Necesidades , Calidad de Vida , Curva ROC , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Traducción
18.
Chem Senses ; 38(3): 251-8, 2013 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23315041

RESUMEN

Several studies have reported that experts outperform novices in specific domains. However, the superiority of experts in accuracy, taking both trueness and precision into consideration, has not yet been explored. Here, we examined differences between expert and novice performances by evaluating the accuracy of their estimations of physical concentrations of sodium chloride in solutions while employing a visual analog scale. In Experiment 1, 14 experts and 13 novices tasted 6 concentrations of the solutions until they had learned their intensities. Subsequently, they repeatedly rated the concentration of 3 other solutions in random order. Although we did not find a difference between the performances of the 2 groups in trueness (difference between rating and correct concentration), the precision (consistency of ratings for each participant) of experts was higher than that of novices. In Experiment 2, 13 experts who had participated in Experiment 1 and 10 experts and 12 novices who had not participated in Experiment 1 rated the salt concentration in sodium chloride/sucrose mixtures in the same way as in Experiment 1. Both trueness and precision of performance were higher in both expert groups than in the novice group. By introducing precision and trueness parameters, we succeeded in quantifying the estimations of experts and novices in rating the concentration of solutions, revealing experts' superiority even for a task they had not been trained for.


Asunto(s)
Cloruro de Sodio/química , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Cloruro de Sodio/farmacología , Umbral Gustativo/efectos de los fármacos
19.
Semin Cell Dev Biol ; 24(3): 247-55, 2013 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23182900

RESUMEN

The perception of taste and flavor can be greatly biased by extrinsic cues, or the information about a food that comes from outside of the food itself, such as package designs, brands, prices, and so on. In order to understand taste/flavor experiences in a broader context, it is necessary to consider factors other than the food/tastants themselves. This review aims to summarize some of the relevant findings from psychological and neuroimaging studies, focusing on depicting how extrinsic cues exert their effect on taste and flavor. Currently, the most frequently considered psychological mediator for the effects of extrinsic cues is expectation. Depending on the gap between expectation and taste/flavor experience, four major models predict outcomes of expectation effects: (1) assimilation, (2) generalized-negativity, (3) contrast, and (4) assimilation-contrast. Among them, the most influential is the assimilation model proposing that taste/flavor experiences are modified toward what one expects. Thus far, all the neuroimaging studies examining the influence of extrinsic cues have dealt with assimilation effects. They suggest that when extrinsic cues influence taste/flavor perception, cortical representations of taste/flavor are also modulated. Collectively neuroimaging findings partly answer questions arising from psychological aspects: the influence of extrinsic cues is not due to superficial response bias but to truly changed perception. These findings, albeit limited to assimilation effects, suggest that combined understanding from both psychological and neuroimaging studies would help deepen our understanding of the taste experience.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Percepción del Gusto , Animales , Señales (Psicología) , Aromatizantes/metabolismo , Humanos , Memoria , Neuroimagen
20.
BMC Med Inform Decis Mak ; 12: 104, 2012 Sep 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22967200

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Health numeracy is an important factor in how well people make decisions based on medical risk information. However, in many countries, including Japan, numeracy studies have been limited. METHODS: To fill this gap, we evaluated health numeracy levels in a sample of Japanese adults by translating two well-known scales that objectively measure basic understanding of math and probability: the 3-item numeracy scale developed by Schwartz and colleagues (the Schwartz scale) and its expanded version, the 11-item numeracy scale developed by Lipkus and colleagues (the Lipkus scale). RESULTS: Participants' performances (n = 300) on the scales were much higher than in original studies conducted in the United States (80% average item-wise correct response rate for Schwartz-J, and 87% for Lipkus-J). This high performance resulted in a ceiling effect on the distributions of both scores, which made it difficult to apply parametric statistical analysis, and limited the interpretation of statistical results. Nevertheless, the data provided some evidence for the reliability and validity of these scales: The reliability of the Japanese versions (Schwartz-J and Lipkus-J) was comparable to the original in terms of their internal consistency (Cronbach's α = 0.53 for Schwartz-J and 0.72 for Lipkus-J). Convergent validity was suggested by positive correlations with an existing Japanese health literacy measure (the Test for Ability to Interpret Medical Information developed by Takahashi and colleagues) that contains some items relevant to numeracy. Furthermore, as shown in the previous studies, health numeracy was still associated with framing bias with individuals whose Lipkus-J performance was below the median being significantly influenced by how probability was framed when they rated surgical risks. A significant association was also found using Schwartz-J, which consisted of only three items. CONCLUSIONS: Despite relatively high levels of health numeracy according to these scales, numeracy measures are still important determinants underlying susceptibility to framing bias. This suggests that it is important in Japan to identify individuals with low numeracy skills so that risk information can be presented in a way that enables them to correctly understand it. Further investigation is required on effective numeracy measures for such an intervention in Japan.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones , Alfabetización en Salud/estadística & datos numéricos , Estado de Salud , Vigilancia de la Población , Medición de Riesgo/métodos , Procedimientos Quirúrgicos Operativos/psicología , Adulto , Anciano , Femenino , Hospitales/estadística & datos numéricos , Humanos , Japón , Funciones de Verosimilitud , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Psicometría , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Medición de Riesgo/normas , Clase Social , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Traducción
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...